负性情绪刺激的反应启动效应事件相关电位的实验研究
The reaction priming effect of emotionally negative stimuli: an ERP study
查看参考文献16篇
文摘
|
目的:许多研究观察到,有机体对负性情绪刺激的反应比对中性或正性刺激快,本研究应用事件相关电位中的单侧化准备电位将反应时划分为刺激加工阶段和反应准备阶段,从而得以直接观察负性刺激的反应启动效应。方法:本研究选取国际情感图片系统的正、中、负性图片作为剌激材料,以正常大学生为被试,令其按键表示对图片情绪属性的判断,同时记录脑电。结果:正、负性情绪刺激引起的刺激锁定单侧化准备电位的潜伏期比中性条件下的潜伏期缩短,观察反应锁定单侧化准备电位的潜伏期可见,负性刺激引起的潜伏期显著短于中性和正性条件。结论:对情绪性刺激的心理加工评价过程较短,观察反应锁定单侧化准备电位的潜伏期可见,负性剌激引起的潜伏期显著短于中性和正性条件,表明负性刺激具有反应启动效应. |
其他语种文摘
|
Objective: There is considerable evidence that organisms react more quickly to emotionally negative stimuli than to neutral or/and positive stimuli. Lateralized readiness potential (LRP) can divide reaction time into a psychological processing stage (stimulus-locked interval. S-LRP onset latency) and a movement readiness stage (response-locked interval, R-LRP onset latency). Utilizing LRP, we can observe the reaction priming effect of negative stimuli directly.Method:The experinlental stimuli were from the International Affeetive Picture System. Eighteen undergraduates participated in this study as paid volunteers. They were told to observe the emotional pictures carefully and press the corresponding buttons as soon as possible to show their judgment of the emotional properties of the pictures.Result:The S-LRP onset latencies of positive and negative stimuli were shorter than that of neutral stimuli. The R-LRP onset latency of negative stimuli was significantly shorter than that of positive and neutral stimuli. Conclusion:The psychological assessment period of emotional events is relatively shorter than that of neutral events. The reaction priming effect of emotionally negative stimuli is observed in this study. |
来源
|
中国康复医学杂志
,2005,20(9):648-651 【扩展库】
|
关键词
|
情绪
;
反应启动效应
;
事件相关电位
;
单侧化准备电位
|
地址
|
北京师范大学, 认知神经科学与学习国家重点实验室, 北京, 100875
|
语种
|
中文 |
文献类型
|
研究性论文 |
ISSN
|
1001-1242 |
学科
|
临床医学;神经病学与精神病学 |
基金
|
国家自然科学基金
;
中国科学院知识创新工程重要方向项目
|
文献收藏号
|
CSCD:2166811
|
参考文献 共
16
共1页
|
1.
Hansen CH. Finding the face in the crowd an anger superiority effect [J].
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,1988,54:917-924
|
CSCD被引
20
次
|
|
|
|
2.
Hman A. The face in the crowd revisitedthreat advantage with schematic stimuli [J].
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,2001,80:381-396
|
CSCD被引
1
次
|
|
|
|
3.
Pratto F. Automatic vigilance the attention-grabbing power of negative social information [J].
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,1991,61:380-391
|
CSCD被引
13
次
|
|
|
|
4.
Taylor SE. Asymmetrical effects of positive and negative eventsthe mobilization-minimization hypothesis [J].
Psychological Bulletin,1991,110:67-85
|
CSCD被引
15
次
|
|
|
|
5.
Coles MGH. Modern mind-brain reading psychophysiolod cognition[J].
Psychophysiology,1989,26:251-269
|
CSCD被引
3
次
|
|
|
|
6.
Rammsayer T. Evidence from lateralized readiness potentials[J].
Biological Psychology,2004,66(1):35-49
|
CSCD被引
2
次
|
|
|
|
7.
Hsieh S. Exploring the nature of switch cost Inferences from P300 and the lateralized readiness potentials [J].
Brain Research Protocols,2003,12:49-59
|
CSCD被引
2
次
|
|
|
|
8.
李心天. 中国人的左右利手分布[J].
心理学报,1983,15(3):268-275
|
CSCD被引
94
次
|
|
|
|
9.
汪向东.
心理卫生评定量表手册(增订版),1999:238-241
|
CSCD被引
33
次
|
|
|
|
10.
Lang PJ. technical manual and affective ratings.
NIMH
|
CSCD被引
1
次
|
|
|
|
11.
黄宇霞. 国际情绪图片系统在中国的试用研究.
中国心理卫生杂志,2004,18(9):631-634
|
CSCD被引
54
次
|
|
|
|
12.
de Jong R. Gratton G.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,1990,16:164-182
|
CSCD被引
1
次
|
|
|
|
13.
Osman A. Temporal organization of covert motor processes during response selection and preparation[J].
Biological Psychology,2003,64:47-75
|
CSCD被引
1
次
|
|
|
|
14.
Lang PJ. The emotion probe studies of motivation and attention[J].
American Psychologist,1995,5:372-385
|
CSCD被引
30
次
|
|
|
|
15.
Miller G. The good.
Science,2002,295:2193-2194
|
CSCD被引
6
次
|
|
|
|
16.
Phillips ML. Neurobiology of Emotion Perception Ⅰthe neural basis of normal emotion perception[J].
Biological Psychiatry,2003,54:504-514
|
CSCD被引
76
次
|
|
|
|
|