汉语学龄前儿童正字法意识的发展
The Development of Orthographic Awareness in Chinese Preschool Children
查看参考文献35篇
文摘
|
本研究选择北京地区幼儿园3岁、4岁、5岁儿童各31、48、33名,采用单部件意识测验和部件位置及功能意识测验系统探查了学龄前儿童的正字法意识各个层面的发展状况。单部件意识测验包括部件替换、部件缺失和部件旋转三个部分;部件位置及功能意识测验分为假字和非字两个部分,而非字又包括两形非字和形声非字。结果发现单部件意识测验中,5岁组儿童在部件替换水平上的得分显著高于3岁组儿童,而3岁组与4岁组、4岁组与5岁组儿童之间无显著差异;部件缺失和部件旋转水平上,三个年龄段的儿童的得分表现出明显的增长趋势。部件位置及功能意识测验中,假字得分在三个年龄段之间无显著差异;非字得分随年龄增长显著提高。这些结果表明,单部件意识在学前期处于不断发展的阶段,其中对部件替换的非字的拒绝能力发展较早,部件缺失和部件旋转非字的拒绝能力发展较晚;部件位置及功能意识在学前期已经开始发展,3岁儿童已经具有假字符合正字法规则的认识,但对非字违反部件位置合法性和功能完整性的认识直到4岁左右才开始萌芽,5岁还未成熟。 |
其他语种文摘
|
The emergent literacy opinion points that children's literacy develops before formal education. Orthographic awareness makes great contribution to Chinese literacy development, and it including three levels: the knowledge of characteristics of a single radical, legal position of a radical and combination rules of two radicals. The present study aimed to reveal the developmental pattern of preschool children's emergent literacy by investigating the development of orthographic awareness in Chinese preschool children systematically with two tests. Radicals are the basic units of Chinese character, so a single radical awareness test was conducted to explore the first level of orthographic awareness, in which children were asked to discriminate three types of non-characters: number replacement, a radical of this character was replaced by a number; radical absence, a radical of the character was missing; radical rotation, one radical of this character was inversed or reversed. The radical position and function awareness test using pseudo-characters and non-characters decision was designed to reveal the development of the latter levels of orthographic awareness. There were two types of non-characters with different structures, one consisting of two semantic radicals, and the other consisting of a semantic radical and a phonetic radical with illegal positions. We recruited 112 preschool children, who fell into three age groups: 3-year-olds, 4-year-olds, and 5-year-olds, respectively consisting of 31, 48 and 33 children. All participants completed both tests. The results of the single radical awareness test showed similar developmental patterns in the radical-absent and radical-rotated non-characters, the scores increased significantly with age; as to the scores of radical-replaced non-characters, significant difference only appeared between 3-year-old children and 5-year-old children. In the radical position and function awareness test, there were no significant differences across age groups in the scores of pseudo-characters, while the scores of non-characters significantly increased with age. Particularly, scores of non-characters with two semantic radicals were significantly higher than non-characters with a semantic radical and a phonetic radical for all three groups. The scores in the two tests were significantly correlated, and the scores in the single radical awareness test were consistently higher than that in radical position and function awareness test for each group children. Results of the single radical awareness test revealed that children's single radical awareness constantly developed in the preschool period. The discrimination in radical-replaced non-characters developed earlier, however, rejection to radical-rotated characters developed later, revealing the development speed varied across aspects of radical knowledge. The knowledge about radical's special function as a script was acquired earlier, while recognition about special form of radical progressed later. Radical position and function awareness also developed in preschool children. Three-year-old children could realize that pseudo-characters obeyed orthographic rules, but knowledge about irregularity of non-characters didn't emerge until 4 years old. Even though there was strong correlation between the single radical awareness and the radical position and function awareness, developmental pattern including developmental speed and order was different. The knowledge of a single radical developed earlier and more quickly than that of radical position and function. |
来源
|
心理学报
,2013,45(1):60-69 【核心库】
|
关键词
|
学龄前
;
汉字
;
部件位置
;
部件功能
;
正字法意识
|
地址
|
中国科学院心理研究所, 中国科学院行为科学重点实验室, 北京, 100101
|
语种
|
中文 |
文献类型
|
研究性论文 |
ISSN
|
0439-755X |
学科
|
社会科学总论 |
基金
|
中国科学院知识创新工程重要方向项目
;
国家自然科学基金面上项目
|
文献收藏号
|
CSCD:4737444
|
参考文献 共
35
共2页
|
1.
Chan L. Children's understanding of the formal and functional characteristics of written Chinese.
Applied Psycholinguistics,1998,19:115-132
|
CSCD被引
1
次
|
|
|
|
2.
陈晓. 儿童家庭读写活动、早期读写水平与小学一年级语文课成绩的关系.
心理发展与教育,2010(3):258-266
|
CSCD被引
3
次
|
|
|
|
3.
Chen Y. What are the functional orthographic units in Chinese word recognition: The stroke or the stroke pattern.
The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A,1996,49(4):1024-1043
|
CSCD被引
1
次
|
|
|
|
4.
Clay M M.
An observation survey of early literacy achievement,1993
|
CSCD被引
1
次
|
|
|
|
5.
Ding G S. The nature of the mental representation of radicals in Chinese: A priming study.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,2004,30(2):530-539
|
CSCD被引
12
次
|
|
|
|
6.
Feldman L B. The role of component function in visual recognition of Chinese characters.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,1997,23(3):776-781
|
CSCD被引
9
次
|
|
|
|
7.
郝美玲. 留学生汉字正字法意识的萌芽与发展.
世界汉语教学,2007,79(1):29-39
|
CSCD被引
1
次
|
|
|
|
8.
Ho C S H. Cognitive profiling and preliminary subtyping in Chinese developmental dyslexia.
Cognition,2004,91(1):43-75
|
CSCD被引
16
次
|
|
|
|
9.
Ho C S H. A" radical" approach to reading development in Chinese: The role of semantic radicals and phonetic radicals.
Journal of Literacy Research,2003,35(3):849-878
|
CSCD被引
3
次
|
|
|
|
10.
Levy B A. Understanding print: Early reading development and the contributions of home literacy experiences.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,2006,93(1):63-93
|
CSCD被引
2
次
|
|
|
|
11.
Leong C K. Enhancing orthographic knowledge helps spelling production in eight-year-old Chinese children at risk for dyslexia.
Annual of Dyslexia,2011,61(1):136-160
|
CSCD被引
2
次
|
|
|
|
12.
黎红. 汉字识别中的部件加工: 错觉性结合实验的证据.
心理科学,1997,22(3):213-217
|
CSCD被引
1
次
|
|
|
|
13.
李虹. 汉语儿童正字法意识的萌芽与发展.
心理发展与教育,2006,22(1):35-38
|
CSCD被引
8
次
|
|
|
|
14.
李娟. 学龄儿童汉语正字法意识发展的研究.
心理学报,2000,32(2):121-126
|
CSCD被引
11
次
|
|
|
|
15.
李燕. 现代汉语形声字声符研究.
现代汉语用字信息分析,1993:84-98
|
CSCD被引
2
次
|
|
|
|
16.
刘鸣. 汉字认知与形象思维.
华南师范大学学报(社会科学版),1996(4):46-51
|
CSCD被引
1
次
|
|
|
|
17.
刘翔平. 儿童识字能力与汉字规则意识关系研究.
中国特殊教育,2006,61(7):56-61
|
CSCD被引
2
次
|
|
|
|
18.
Lomax R G. Young children's concepts about print and reading: Toward a model of word reading acquisition.
Reading Research Quarterly,1987,22(2):237-256
|
CSCD被引
1
次
|
|
|
|
19.
Lonigan C J. Development of emergent literacy and early reading skills in preschool children: Evidence from a latent-variable longitudinal study.
Developmental Psychology,2000,36(5):596
|
CSCD被引
3
次
|
|
|
|
20.
Luo Y C. Development of Chinese orthographic processing A cross-cultural perspective.
Writing System Research,2011,3(1):69-86
|
CSCD被引
1
次
|
|
|
|
|